Importance Of Verbal Humor

830 Words2 Pages

Language is constantly changing, evolving, embracing new words, and adapting to the needs of the speaker. There is a big discrepancy between the epic spoken narrations of Homer and the wit of Shakespearean works, as well as between the texts of the latter and the language we use today. Dutton (2003: 4) concludes that mankind has been telling jokes ab initio. Despite its long history of formation, verbal humor is observed as a means of understanding the production and comprehension of language. As noted by Pinker (1999: 48), wordplay employs the mind’s metalinguistic abilities: it is the way of using language to deconstruct and critique itself. Puns in particular bring significant changes into the study of linguistics. Whether they are enjoyed
Freud (1905) himself found much to analyze about verbal humor. He refined all humor through the never-ending war between the id, ego and superego, distinguishing verbal humor from its cousins, as a form of “judgment”, which may highlight and blend together separate ideas. Like much of his theory, Freud’s attempt to fit all comedy into a psychoanalytic frame has been heavily criticized since its publishing. Psychologists have been judging Freud's classification of humor as inappropriate and lacking empirical support.
According to Sohval (2011: 16) using the same material for several times is an essential technique for producing verbal jokes. Obviously, he refers to nothing other but a word capable of multiple interpretations.
Dienhart (1999: 81) agrees with Freud’s supposition regarding puns’ idea-linking properties. In his essay he ascribes the enjoyment drawn from jokes, particularly puns. Dienhart (1999: 134) also insists that good verbal humor renders the phonological likenesses and semantic differences between words in a text. What is more, he suggests that good comedy is a result of our comparing multiple ideas, similar to how we compare one thing to another or behave according to certain rules we
GraPHIA is a connectionist model made up of “a semantic network and a phonological network where words are represented by nodes in both the networks” (3). According to what Dienhart (1999: 93) says, verbal humor turns around a perceived ambiguity between phonology and semantics. Similarly, Srinivasan and Pariyadath (2008) highlighted three main requirements necessary for the perception of humor: “violation of a moral commitment the perceiver holds important, normalcy perceived in the situation, and simultaneity of both the normalcy and violation” (5). In their model they presume the recipient has already achieved some results in processing the joke semantically and phonologically enough to analyze the relevant words. According to this midel, the “violation” element is actually important to the perception of humor, and the “relevant” words are compared between the semantic and phonological networks. Between these two networks there are a few weighted connections, with stronger weights representing the dissemblance of the words, allowing weights to detect a meaning or sound violation. In case of detection of a violation, all possible homophones are run through an algorithm that establishes if a pun is possibly based on the homophones and have semantic

Open Document